Advertisement

Individual Differences in the Effects of Music Engagement on Responses to Painful Stimulation

Published:November 10, 2011DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2011.08.010

      Abstract

      Engaged attention, including music listening, has shown mixed results when used as a method for reducing pain. Applying the framework of constructivism, we extend the concept of engagement beyond attention/distraction to include all cognitive and emotional/motivational processes that may be recruited in order to construct an alternative experience to pain and thus reduce pain. Using a music-listening task varying in task demand, we collected stimulus-evoked potentials, pupil dilation, and skin conductance responses to noxious electrocutaneous stimulations as indicators of central and peripheral arousal, respectively. Trait anxiety (Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory) and absorption (Tellegen Absorption Scale) provided indicators of individual differences. One hundred and fifty-three healthy, normal volunteers participated in a test session in which they received 3 stimulus intensity levels while listening to background tones (No Task) or performing a music-listening task. Linear slopes indicating net engagement (change in stimulus arousal relative to task performance) decreased with increasing task demand and stimulus level for stimulus-evoked potentials. Slopes for pupil dilation response and skin conductance response varied with task demand, anxiety, and absorption, with the largest engagement effect occurring for high anxiety/high absorption participants. Music engagement reduces pain responses, but personality factors like anxiety and absorption modulate the magnitude of effect.

      Perspective

      Engaging in music listening can reduce responses to pain, depending on the person: people who are anxious and can become absorbed in activities easily may find music listening especially effective for relieving pain. Clinicians should consider patients’ personality characteristics when recommending behavioral interventions like music listening for pain relief.

      Key words

      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic and Personal

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to The Journal of Pain
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      References

        • Bach D.R.
        • Flandin G.
        • Friston K.J.
        • Dolan R.J.
        Time-series analysis for rapid event-related skin conductance responses.
        J Neurosci Methods. 2009; 184: 224-234
        • Bishop S.J.
        Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2007; 11: 307-316
        • Bromm B.
        • Meier W.
        The intracutaneous stimulus: A new pain model for algesimetric studies.
        Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 1984; 6: 405-410
        • Buck R.
        • Morley S.
        A daily process design study of attentional pain control strategies in the self-management of cancer pain.
        Eur J Pain. 2006; 10: 385-398
        • Buhle J.
        • Wager T.D.
        Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task.
        Pain. 2010; 149: 19-26
        • Bushnell M.C.
        • Villemure C.
        • Duncan G.H.
        Psychophysical and neurophysiological studies of pain modulation by attention.
        in: Price D.D. Bushnell M.C. Psychological Methods of Pain Control: Basic Science and Clinical Perspectives. IASP Press, Seattle, WA2004: 99-116
        • Campbell C.M.
        • Witmer K.
        • Simango M.
        • Carteret A.
        • Loggia M.L.
        • Campbell J.N.
        • Haythornthwaite J.A.
        • Edwards R.R.
        Catastrophizing delays the analgesic effect of distraction.
        Pain. 2010; 149: 202-207
      1. Cepeda M, Carr D, Lau J, Alvarez H: Music for pain relief. Cochrane Database Syst Rev:CD004843, 2006

        • Chapman C.R.
        • Nakamura Y.
        Hypnotic analgesia: A constructivist framework.
        Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1998; 46: 6-27
        • Chen A.C.
        • Arendt-Nielsen L.
        • Plaghkl L.
        Laser-evoked potentials in human pain: I. Use and possible misuse.
        Pain Forum. 1998; 7: 174-184
        • Critchley H.D.
        • Elliott R.
        • Mathias C.J.
        • Dolan R.J.
        Neural activity relating to generation and representation of galvanic skin conductance responses: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
        J Neurosci. 2000; 20: 3033-3040
        • Crombez G.
        • Eccleston C.
        • Baeyens F.
        • Eelen P.
        Attentional disruption is enhanced by the threat of pain.
        Behav Res Ther. 1998; 36: 195-204
        • Donaldson G.W.
        • Chapman C.R.
        • Nakamura Y.
        • Bradshaw D.H.
        • Jacobson R.C.
        • Chapman C.N.
        Pain and the defense response: Structural equation modeling reveals a coordinated psychophysiological response to increasing painful stimulation.
        Pain. 2003; 102: 97-108
        • Eccleston C.
        • Crombez G.
        Pain demands attention: A cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain.
        Psychol Bull. 1999; 125: 356-366
        • Hodes R.L.
        • Howland E.W.
        • Lightfoot N.
        • Cleeland C.S.
        The effects of distraction on responses to cold pressor pain.
        Pain. 1990; 41: 109-114
        • James J.E.
        • Hardardottir D.
        Influence of attention focus and trait anxiety on tolerance of acute pain.
        Br J Health Psychol. 2002; 7: 149-162
        • Janssen S.A.
        • Arntz A.
        Anxiety and pain: Attentional and endorphinergic influences.
        Pain. 1996; 66: 145-150
        • Johnson M.H.
        How does distraction work in the management of pain?.
        Curr Pain Headache Rep. 2005; 9: 90-95
        • Kajimura N.
        • Kato M.
        • Sekimoto M.
        • Watanabe T.
        • Takahashi K.
        • Okuma T.
        • Mizuki Y.
        • Yamada M.
        A polysomnographic study of sleep patterns in normal humans with low- or high-anxiety personality traits.
        Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998; 52: 317-320
        • Koelsch S.
        • Siebel W.A.
        Towards a neural basis of music perception.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9: 578-584
        • Kohut H.
        • Levarie S.
        On the enjoyment of listening to music.
        Psychoanal Q. 1950; 19: 64-87
        • Kwekkeboom K.
        • Huseby-Moore K.
        • Ward S.
        Imaging ability and effective use of guided imagery.
        Res Nurs Health. 1998; 21: 189-198
        • Lang P.J.
        • Bradley M.M.
        • Cuthbert B.N.
        Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex.
        Psychol Rev. 1990; 97: 377-395
        • Legrain V.
        • Damme S.V.
        • Eccleston C.
        • Davis K.D.
        • Seminowicz D.A.
        • Crombez G.
        A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence.
        Pain. 2009; 144: 230-232
        • Legrain V.
        • Crombez G.
        • Verhoeven K.
        • Mouraux A.
        The role of working memory in the attentional control of pain.
        Pain. 2011; 152: 453-459
        • Lykken D.T.
        • Macindoe I.
        • Tellegen A.
        Perception: Autonomic response to shock as a function of predictability in time and locus.
        Psychophysiol. 1972; 9: 318-333
        • McCaffrey R.
        Music listening: Its effects in creating a healing environment.
        J Psychosocial Nurs Ment Health Serv. 2008; 46: 39-44
        • McCaffrey R.G.
        • Good M.
        The lived experience of listening to music while recovering from surgery.
        J Holist Nurs. 2000; 18: 378-390
        • Mercado F.
        • Carretie L.
        • Hinojosa J.A.
        • Penacoba C.
        Two successive phases in the threat-related attentional response of anxious subjects: Neural correlates.
        Depress Anxiety. 2009; 26: 1141-1150
        • Mitchell L.A.
        • MacDonald R.A.
        An experimental investigation of the effects of preferred and relaxing music listening on pain perception.
        J Music Ther. 2006; 43: 295-316
        • O’Hare A.J.
        • Dien J.
        The Fear Survey Schedule as a measure of anxious arousal: Evidence from ERPs.
        Neurosci Lett. 2008; 441: 243-247
        • Platt R.D.
        • Lacey S.C.
        • Iobst A.D.
        • Finkelman D.
        Absorption, dissociation, and fantasy-proneness as predictors of memory distortion in autobiographical and laboratory-generated memories.
        Appl Cogn Psychol. 1998; 12: S77-S89
        • Roy M.
        • Peretz I.
        • Rainville P.
        Emotional valence contributes to music-induced analgesia.
        Pain. 2008; 134: 140-147
        • Seminowicz D.A.
        • Davis K.D.
        Cortical responses to pain in healthy individuals depends on pain catastrophizing.
        Pain. 2006; 120: 297-306
        • Seminowicz D.A.
        • Davis K.D.
        Interactions of pain intensity and cognitive load: The brain stays on task.
        Cereb Cortex. 2007; 17: 1412-1422
        • Spielberger C.D.
        Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (Form Y).
        Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Palo Alto, CA1970
        • Stowell H.
        Event related brain potentials and human pain: A first objective overview.
        Int J Psychophysiol. 1984; 1: 137-151
        • Tellegen A.
        • Atkinson G.
        Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility.
        J Abnorm Psychol. 1974; 83: 268-277
        • Van Damme S.
        • Crombez G.
        • Eccleston C.
        • Koster E.H.
        Hypervigilance to learned pain signals: A componential analysis.
        J Pain. 2006; 7: 346-357
        • Van Damme S.
        • Crombez G.
        • Van Nieuwenborgh-De Wever K.
        • Goubert L.
        Is distraction less effective when pain is threatening? An experimental investigation with the cold pressor task.
        Eur J Pain. 2008; 12: 60-67
        • Van Damme S.
        • Legrain V.
        • Vogt J.
        • Crombez G.
        Keeping pain in mind: A motivational account of attention to pain.
        Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010; 34: 204-213
        • Verhoeven K.
        • Crombez G.
        • Eccleston C.
        • Van Ryckeghem D.M.
        • Morley S.
        • Van Damme S.
        The role of motivation in distracting attention away from pain: An experimental study.
        Pain. 2010; 149: 229-234
        • Verney S.P.
        • Granholm E.
        • Dionisio D.P.
        Pupillary responses and processing resources on the visual backward masking task.
        Psychophysiology. 2001; 38: 76-83
        • Wadsworth B.J.
        Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive and Affective Development: Foundations of Constructivism.
        Longman Publishing, New York, NY1995
        • Zaslansky R.
        • Sprecher E.
        • Katz Y.
        • Rozenberg B.
        • Hemli J.A.
        • Yarnitsky D.
        Pain-evoked potentials: What do they really measure?.
        Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996; 100: 384-391