Advertisement

Patients Are Socially Excluded When Their Pain Has No Medical Explanation

      Highlights

      • Observers were less willing to interact with patients with unexplained pain.
      • Observers less often selected patients with unexplained pain to play a game with.
      • Observers attributed less pain to patients with unexplained pain.

      Abstract

      This study investigated whether observers socially exclude patients who experience pain that is not medically explained by means of an experimental design. Fifty-nine participants (individuals from the general population) viewed videos of 4 patients, each accompanied by a vignette describing the presence or absence of a medical explanation for their pain. Participants estimated patient's pain, and rated the sympathy felt for and the inclination to help the patient. To measure social exclusion, participants indicated their willingness to interact with the patients in several situations (Social Distance Scale). Furthermore, the participants were invited to select 2 of the 4 patients as confederates to play a game against another duo. When no medical explanation for the pain was provided, participants attributed less pain, reported feeling less sympathy, and were less inclined to help the patients with daily activities. Of particular importance to this study, participants were less willing to interact with patients with medically unexplained pain and selected less often patients with ‘medically unexplained’ pain than patients with ‘medically explained’ pain as confederates in the social game. These results are indicative of social exclusion of patients with pain for which there is no clear medical explanation.

      Perspective

      Observers socially exclude patients with pain for which there is no clear medical explanation. These findings have important clinical implications. In particular, social exclusion might have detrimental effects on the mental and physical well-being of patients with pain.

      Key words

      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic and Personal

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to The Journal of Pain
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      References

        • Allport G.W.
        The nature of prejudice.
        Addison-Wesley, Reading1954
        • Birtane M.
        • Uzunca K.
        • Taştekin N.
        • Tuna H.
        The evaluation of quality of life in fibromyalgia syndrome: A comparison with rheumatoid arthritis by using SF-36 Health Survey.
        Clin Rheumatol. 2007; 26: 679-684
        • Blascovich J.
        • Mendes W.
        • Hunter S.
        • Lickel B.
        Stigma, threat, and social interactions.
        in: Heatherthon T.F. Hebl M.R. Kleck R.E. Hull J.G. The Social Psychology of Stigma. Guilford Press, New York2000: 307-333
        • Borenstein M.
        • Hedges L.V.
        • Higgings J.P.
        • Rothstein H.R.
        Introduction to Meta-Analysis.
        Wiley, West Sussex2009
        • Caes L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Devos P.
        • Verlooy J.
        • Benoit Y.
        • Goubert L.
        Parental distress and catastrophic thoughts about child pain: Implications for parental protective behavior in the context of child leukemia-related medical procedures.
        Clin J Pain. 2014; 30: 787-799
        • Chambers L.A.
        • Rueda S.
        • Baker D.N.
        • Wilson M.G.
        • Deutsch R.
        • Raeifar E.
        • Rourke S.B.
        Stigma, HIV and health: A qualitative synthesis.
        BMC Public Health. 2015; 15: 848
        • Chibnall J.T.
        • Tait R.C.
        Observer perceptions of low back pain - effects of pain report and other contextual factors.
        J Appl Soc Psychol. 1995; 25: 418-439
        • Chibnall J.T.
        • Tait R.C.
        • Ross L.R.
        The effects of medical evidence and pain intensity on medical student judgments of chronic pain patients.
        J Behav Med. 1997; 20: 257-271
        • Corrigan P.W.
        • Kerr A.
        • Knudsen L.
        The stigma of mental illness: Explanatory models and methods for change.
        Appl Prev Psychol. 2005; 11: 179-190
        • Cosmides L.
        The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task.
        Cognition. 1989; 31: 187-276
        • Craig K.D.
        Social communication model of pain.
        Pain. 2015; 156: 1198-1199
        • Craig K.D.
        • Badali M.A.
        Introduction to the special series on pain deception and malingering.
        Clin J Pain. 2004; 20: 377-382
        • Craig K.D.
        • Hill M.L.
        • McMurtry B.
        Detecting deception and malingering.
        in: Block A.R. Kremer E.F. Fernandez E. Handbook of Pain Syndromes: Biopsychosocial Perspectives. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ1999: 41-58
        • Crocker J.
        • Major B.
        • Steele C.
        Social Stigma.
        in: Gilbert T. Fiske S. Lindzey G. The Handbook of Social Psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford1998: 504-553
        • Crombie I.K.
        • Croft P.R.
        • Linton S.J.
        • LeResche L.
        • Von Korff M.
        Epidemiology of Pain.
        IASP Press, Seattle1991
        • Dagenais S.
        • Caro J.
        • Haldeman S.
        A systematic review of low back pain cost of illness studies in the United States and internationally.
        Spine J. 2008; 8: 8-20
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Craig K.D.
        Understanding stigma and chronic pain: A state of the art review.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 1607-1610
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Goubert L.
        • Stevens M.
        • Amanda A.C.
        • Crombez G.
        Discounting pain in the absence of medical evidence is explained by negative evaluation of the patient.
        Pain. 2013; 154: 669-676
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Goubert L.
        • Stevens M.A.
        • Deveugele M.
        • Craig K.D.
        • Crombez G.
        Health care professionals’ reactions to patient pain: Impact of knowledge about medical evidence and psychosocial influences.
        J Pain. 2014; 15: 262-270
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Goubert L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Prkachin K.M.
        • Crombez G.
        We discount the pain of others when pain has no medical explanation.
        J Pain. 2012; 13: 1198-1205
        • Dewar A.
        • White M.
        • Posade S.T.
        • Dillon W.
        Using nominal group technique to assess chronic pain, patients’ perceived challenges and needs in a community health region.
        Heal Expect. 2003; 6: 44-52
        • Dunlap W.P.
        • Cortina J.M.
        • Vaslow J.B.
        • Burke M.J.
        Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs.
        Psychol Methods. 1996; 1: 170-177
        • Eccleston C.
        • Crombez G.
        Worry and chronic pain: A misdirected problem solving model.
        Pain. 2007; 132: 233-236
        • Eisenberger N.I.
        • Lieberman M.D.
        Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2004; 8: 294-300
        • Glenton C.
        Chronic back pain sufferers - striving for the sick role.
        Soc Sci Med. 2003; 57: 2243-2252
        • Goldberg D.S.
        • McGee S.J.
        Pain as a global public health priority.
        BMC Public Health. 2011; 11: 770
        • Goubert L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Cano A.
        • Crombez G.
        Catastrophizing about their children’s pain is related to higher parent-child congruency in pain ratings: An experimental investigation.
        Eur J Pain. 2009; 13: 196-201
        • Hartgerink C.H.
        • van Beest I.
        • Wicherts J.M.
        • Williams K.D.
        The ordinal effects of ostracism: A meta-analysis of 120 cyberball studies.
        PLoS One. 2015; 10: e0127002
        • Hiller W.
        • Rief W.
        • Brähler E.
        Somatization in the population: From mild bodily misperceptions to disabling symptoms.
        Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2006; 41: 704-712
        • Holloway I.
        • Sofaer-Bennett B.
        • Walker J.
        The stigmatisation of people with chronic back pain.
        Disabil Rehabil. 2007; 29: 1456-1464
        • Kappesser J.
        • de C Williams A.C.
        Pain judgements of patients’ relatives: Examining the use of social contract theory as theoretical framework.
        J Behav Med. 2008; 31: 309-317
        • Kool M.B.
        • van Middendorp H.
        • Boeije H.R.
        • Geenen R.
        Understanding the lack of understanding: Invalidation from the perspective of the patient with fibromyalgia.
        Arthritis Rheum. 2009; 61: 1650-1656
        • Kool M.B.
        • van Middendorp H.
        • Lumley M.A.
        • Schenk Y.
        • Jacobs J.W.
        • Bijlsma J.W.
        • Geenen R.
        Lack of understanding in fibromyalgia and rheumatoid arthritis: The Illness Invalidation Inventory (3*I).
        Ann Rheum Dis. 2010; 69: 1990-1995
        • Lahmann C.
        • Henningsen P.
        • Dinkel A.
        Somatoform disorders and functional somatic syndromes [in German].
        Neurologist. 2010; 81: 1383-1396
        • Link B.G.
        • Cullen F.T.
        • Frank J.
        • Wozniak J.F.
        The social rejection of former mental patients: Understanding why labels matter.
        Am J Sociol. 1987; 92: 1461-1500
        • Link B.G.
        • Yang L.H.
        • Phelan J.C.
        • Collins P.Y.
        Measuring mental illness stigma.
        Schizophr Bull. 2004; 30: 511-541
        • Macdonald G.
        • Leary M.R.
        Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain.
        Psychol Bull. 2005; 131: 202-223
        • Major B.
        • Eccleston C.
        Stigma and social exclusion.
        in: Abrams D. Hogg M.A. Marques J.M. The Social Pscychology Inclusion and Exclusion. Psychology Press, New York2005: 63-87
        • Neuberg S.
        • Smith D.
        • Asher T.
        Why people stigmatize: Toward a biocultural framework.
        in: Heatherthon T.F. Hebl M.R. Kleck R.E. Hull J.G. The Social Psychology of Stigma. Guilford Press, New York2000: 31-61
        • Newton B.J.
        • Southall J.L.
        • Raphael J.H.
        • Ashford R.L.
        • LeMarchand K.
        A narrative review of the impact of disbelief in chronic pain.
        Pain Manag Nurs. 2013; 14: 161-171
        • Pedersen R.
        Empirical research on empathy in medicine—A critical review.
        Patient Educ Couns. 2009; 76: 307-322
        • Puhl R.
        • Suh Y.
        Stigma and eating and weight disorders.
        Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2015; 17: 10
        • Reid K.J.
        • Harker J.
        • Bala M.M.
        • Truyers C.
        • Kellen E.
        • Bekkering G.E.
        • Kleijnen J.
        Epidemiology of chronic non-cancer pain in Europe: Narrative review of prevalence, pain treatments and pain impact.
        Curr Med Res Opin. 2011; 27: 449-462
        • Stokes J.
        • Evans C.J.
        • Pompilus F.
        • Shields A.L.
        • Summers K.H.
        Development of a questionnaire to assess the impact of chronic low back pain for use in regulated clinical trials.
        Patient. 2013; 6: 291-305
        • Stone L.
        Blame, shame and hopelessness: Medically unexplained symptoms and the “heartsink” experience.
        Aust Fam Physician. 2014; 43: 191-195
        • Tait R.C.
        • Chibnall J.T.
        Observer perceptions of chronic low back pain.
        J Appl Soc Psychol. 1994; 24: 415-431
        • Tait R.C.
        • Chibnall J.T.
        Physician judgments of chronic pain patients.
        Soc Sci Med. 1997; 45: 1199-1205
        • Toye F.
        • Barker K.
        “Could I be imagining this?” - The dialectic struggles of people with persistent unexplained back pain.
        Disabil Rehabil. 2010; 32: 1722-1732
        • Toye F.
        • Barker K.
        Persistent non-specific low back pain and patients’ experience of general practice: A qualitative study.
        Prim Health Care Res Dev. 2012; 13: 72-84
        • Twenge J.M.
        • Baumeister R.F.
        • DeWall C.N.
        • Ciarocco N.J.
        • Bartels J.M.
        Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior.
        J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007; 92: 56-66
        • VanBeest I.
        • Carter-Sowell A.R.
        • van Dijk E.
        • Williams K.D.
        Groups being ostracized by groups: Is the pain shared, is recovery quicker, and are groups more likely to be aggressive?.
        Group Dyn. 2012; 16: 241-254
        • Walker E.
        • Keegan D.
        • Gardner G.
        • Sullivan M.
        • Katon W.
        • Bernstein D.
        Psychosocial factors in fibromyalgia compared with rheumatoid arthritis .1. Psychiatric diagnoses and functional disability.
        Psychosom Med. 1997; 59: 565-571
        • Watkins E.A.
        • Wollan P.C.
        • Melton L.J.
        • Yawn B.P.
        A population in pain: Report from the Olmsted County health study.
        Pain Med. 2008; 9: 166-174
        • Werner A.
        • Malterud K.
        It is hard work behaving as a credible patient: Encounters between women with chronic pain and their doctors.
        Soc Sci Med. 2003; 57: 1409-1419
        • Williams A.C.
        Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2002; 25 (discussion: 455-488): 439-455
        • Wilson S.J.
        • Martire L.M.
        • Keefe F.J.
        • Mogle J.A.
        • Stephens M.A.
        • Schulz R.
        Daily verbal and nonverbal expression of osteoarthritis pain and spouse responses.
        Pain. 2013; 154: 2045-2053