Advertisement

The Influence of Social Threat on Pain, Aggression, and Empathy in Women

  • Kai Karos
    Correspondence
    Address reprint requests to Kai Karos, Msc, Department of Psychology, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, Box 3726, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
    Affiliations
    Research Group of Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
    Search for articles by this author
  • Ann Meulders
    Affiliations
    Research Group of Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

    Center for Excellence on Generalization Research in Health and Psychopathology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
    Search for articles by this author
  • Liesbet Goubert
    Affiliations
    Department of Experimental-Clinical and Health Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
    Search for articles by this author
  • Johan W.S. Vlaeyen
    Affiliations
    Research Group of Health Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

    Center for Excellence on Generalization Research in Health and Psychopathology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

    Department of Clinical Psychological Sciences, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
    Search for articles by this author
Published:November 21, 2017DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2017.11.003

      Highlights

      • Social threat is associated with increased threat value of pain.
      • Social threat also increases aggression and reduces empathy toward others.
      • Social threat did not affect painful facial expression.
      • Social threat also did not affect pain intensity or unpleasantness ratings.

      Abstract

      Only one published study has investigated the effect of a threatening social context on the perception and expression of pain, showing that social threat leads to increased pain reports but reduced nonverbal pain expression. The current study aimed to replicate and extend these findings to further explore the effects of a threatening social context. Healthy, female participants (N = 71) received 10 electrocutaneous stimuli delivered by a confederate. They were led to believe that the confederate was requested to administer 10 painful stimuli (control group) or that the confederate deliberately chose to deliver 10 painful stimuli when given the choice to deliver between 1 to 10 painful stimuli (social threat group). Self-reported pain intensity, unpleasantness, threat value of pain, and painful facial expression were assessed. Additionally, empathy and aggression toward the confederate were investigated. Social threat did not affect painful facial expression or self-reported pain intensity, but led to increased aggression toward the confederate. Moreover, perceived social threat predicted the threat value of pain and reduced empathy toward the confederate. We were not able to replicate the previously reported dissociation between pain reports and pain expression as a result of social threat. However, social threat was associated with an increased threat value of pain, increased aggression, and reduced empathy.

      Perspective

      A threatening social context affects how threatening pain is perceived and has interpersonal consequences such as increased aggression and reduced empathy, thereby creating a double burden on the individual suffering from pain.

      Key words

      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      Purchase one-time access:

      Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online access
      One-time access price info
      • For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
      • For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'

      Subscribe:

      Subscribe to The Journal of Pain
      Already a print subscriber? Claim online access
      Already an online subscriber? Sign in
      Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect

      References

        • Anderson C.A.
        • Bushman B.J.
        Human aggression.
        Annu Rev Psychol. 2002; 53: 27-51
        • Batson C.
        The altruism question: Towards a social-psychological answer.
        Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ1991
        • Caes L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Trost Z.
        • Goubert L.
        Impact of parental catastrophizing and contextual threat on parents' emotional and behavioral responses to their child's pain.
        Pain. 2012; 153: 687-695
        • Craig K.D.
        Social communication model of pain.
        Pain. 2015; 156: 1198-1199
        • de C Williams A.C.
        • Craig K.D.
        A science of pain expression?.
        Pain. 2006; 125: 202-203
        • de Williams A.C.
        Defeating the stigma of chronic pain.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 1581-1582
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Craig K.D.
        Understanding stigma and chronic pain.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 1607-1610
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Goubert L.
        • Stevens M.
        • Williams A.C.
        • Crombez G.
        Discounting pain in the absence of medical evidence is explained by negative evaluation of the patient.
        Pain. 2013; 154: 669-676
        • De Ruddere L.
        • Goubert L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Kappesser J.
        • Crombez G.
        Impact of being primed with social deception upon observer responses to others' pain.
        Pain. 2013; 154: 221-226
        • Ekman P.
        • Friesen V, W.
        Facial action coding system.
        Consulting Psychologists, Palo Alto1978
        • Fekkes M.
        • Pijpers F.I.
        • Fredriks A.M.
        • Vogels T.
        • Verloove-Vanhorick S.P.
        Do bullied children get ill, or do ill children get bullied? A prospective cohort study on the relationship between bullying and health-related symptoms.
        Pediatrics. 2006; 117: 1568-1574
        • Giancola P.R.
        • Parrott D.J.
        Further evidence for the validity of the Taylor aggression paradigm.
        Aggress Behav. 2008; 34: 214-229
        • Gini G.
        • Pozzoli T.
        Association between bullying and psychosomatic problems: A meta-analysis.
        Pediatrics. 2009; 123: 1059-1065
        • Goubert L.
        • Craig K.D.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Morley S.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • de C Williams A.C.
        • Cano A.
        • Crombez G.
        Facing others in pain: The effects of empathy.
        Pain. 2005; 118: 285-288
        • Goubert L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Verhoeven K.
        • Crombez G.
        Parental emotional responses to their child's pain: The role of dispositional empathy and catastrophizing about their child's pain.
        J Pain. 2008; 9: 272-279
        • Gray K.
        • Wegner D.M.
        The sting of intentional pain.
        Psychol Sci. 2008; 19: 1260-1262
        • Grégoire M.
        • Coll M.P.
        • Tremblay M.P.
        • Prkachin K.M.
        • Jackson P.L.
        Repeated exposure to others' pain reduces vicarious pain intensity estimation.
        Eur J Pain. 2016; 20: 1-9
        • Hadjistavropoulos T.
        • Craig K.D.
        • Duck S.
        • Cano A.
        • Goubert L.
        • Jackson P.L.
        • Mogil J.S.
        • Rainville P.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • de C Williams A.C.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Fitzgerald T.D.
        A biopsychosocial formulation of pain communication.
        Psychol Bull. 2011; 137: 910-939
        • Holm S.
        A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure.
        Scand J Stat. 1979; 6: 65-70
        • Hsieh A.Y.
        • Tripp D.A.
        • Ji L.J.
        The influence of ethnic concordance and discordance on verbal reports and nonverbal behaviours of pain.
        Pain. 2011; 152: 2016-2022
        • Kappesser J.
        • Williams A.C.
        • Prkachin K.M.
        Testing two accounts of pain underestimation.
        Pain. 2006; 124: 109-116
        • Karos K.
        Hell is other people: On the importance of social context in pain research.
        European Health Psychologist. 2017; 19: 290-296
        • Karos K.
        • Meulders A.
        • Vlaeyen J.W.
        Threatening social context facilitates pain-related fear learning.
        J Pain. 2015; 16: 214-225
        • Keogh E.
        Gender differences in the nonverbal communication of pain: A new direction for sex, gender, and pain research?.
        Pain. 2014; 155: 1927-1931
        • Krahé C.
        • Springer A.
        • Weinman J.A.
        • Fotopoulou A.
        The social modulation of pain: Others as predictive signals of salience—A systematic review.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7: 386
        • Krämer U.M.
        • Jansma H.
        • Tempelmann C.
        • Münte T.F.
        Tit-for-tat: The neural basis of reactive aggression.
        Neuroimage. 2007; 38: 203-211
        • Kunz M.
        • Lautenbacher S.
        The faces of pain: A cluster analysis of individual differences in facial activity patterns of pain.
        Eur J Pain. 2014; 18: 813-823
        • Lindsay J.J.
        • Anderson C.
        From antecedent conditions to violent actions: A general affective aggression model.
        Personal Soc Psychol Bull. 2000; 26: 533-547
        • Lissek S.
        • Pine D.S.
        • Grillon C.
        The strong situation: A potential impediment to studying the psychobiology and pharmacology of anxiety disorders.
        Biol Psychol. 2006; 72: 265-270
        • Miller D.T.
        Disrespect and the experience of injustice.
        Annu Rev Psychol. 2001; 52: 527-553
        • Peeters P.A.
        • Vlaeyen J.W.
        Feeling more pain, yet showing less: The influence of social threat on pain.
        J Pain. 2011; 12: 1255-1261
        • Prkachin K.M.
        Assessing pain by facial expression: Facial expression as nexus.
        Pain Res Manag. 2009; 14: 53-58
        • Prkachin K.M.
        The consistency of facial expressions of pain: A comparison across modalities.
        Pain. 2012; 51: 297-306
        • Prkachin K.M.
        • Rocha E.M.
        High levels of vicarious exposure bias pain judgments.
        J Pain. 2010; 11: 904-909
        • Schäfer G.
        • Prkachin K.M.
        • Kaseweter K.A.
        • de C Williams A.C.
        Health care providers' judgments in chronic pain.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 1618-1625
        • Scott W.
        • Trost Z.
        • Bernier E.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        Anger differentially mediates the relationship between perceived injustice and chronic pain outcomes.
        Pain. 2013; 154: 1691-1698
        • Spruyt A.
        • Clarysse J.
        • Vansteenwegen D.
        • Baeyens F.
        • Hermans D.
        Affect 4.0: A free software package for implementing psychological and psychophysiological experiments.
        Exp Psychol. 2009; 57: 36-45
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Adams H.
        • Horan S.
        • Maher D.
        • Boland D.
        • Gross R.
        The role of perceived injustice in the experience of chronic pain and disability: Scale development and validation.
        J Occup Rehabil. 2008; 18: 249-261
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Bishop S.R.
        • Pivik J.
        The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation.
        Psychol Assess. 1995; 7: 524-532
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Martel M.O.
        • Tripp D.
        • Savard A.
        • Crombez G.
        The relation between catastrophizing and the communication of pain experience.
        Pain. 2006; 122: 282-288
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Scott W.
        • Trost Z.
        Perceived injustice: A risk factor for problematic pain outcomes.
        Clin J Pain. 2012; 28: 484-488
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Thibault P.
        • Simmonds M.J.
        • Milioto M.
        • Cantin A.P.
        • Velly A.M.
        Pain, perceived injustice and the persistence of post-traumatic stress symptoms during the course of rehabilitation for whiplash injuries.
        Pain. 2009; 145: 325-331
        • Tremblay P.F.
        • Belchevski M.
        Did the instigator intend to provoke? A key moderator in the relation between trait aggression and aggressive behavior.
        Aggress Behav. 2004; 30: 409-424
        • Trost Z.
        • Vangronsveld K.
        • Linton S.J.
        • Quartana P.J.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        Cognitive dimensions of anger in chronic pain.
        Pain. 2012; 153: 515-517
        • Vervoort T.
        • Caes L.
        • Trost Z.
        • Sullivan M.
        • Vangronsveld K.
        • Goubert L.
        Social modulation of facial pain display in high-catastrophizing children: An observational study in schoolchildren and their parents.
        Pain. 2011; 152: 1591-1599
        • Vlaeyen J.W.
        • Hanssen M.
        • Goubert L.
        • Vervoort T.
        • Peters M.
        • van Breukelen G.
        • Sullivan M.J.
        • Morley S.
        Threat of pain influences social context effects on verbal pain report and facial expression.
        Behav Res Ther. 2009; 47: 774-782
        • Voerman J.S.
        • Vogel I.
        • De Waart F.
        • Westendorp T.
        • Timman R.
        • Busschbach V, J.J.
        • Van De Looij-Jansen P.
        • De Klerk C.
        Bullying, abuse and family conflict as risk factors for chronic pain among Dutch adolescents.
        Eur J Pain. 2015; 19: 1544-1551
        • Walsh J.
        • Eccleston C.
        • Keogh E.
        Pain communication through body posture: The development and validation of a stimulus set.
        Pain. 2014; 155: 2282-2290
        • Warburton W.A.
        • Williams K.D.
        • Cairns D.R.
        When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation.
        J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006; 42: 213-220
        • Waugh O.C.
        • Byrne D.G.
        • Nicholas M.K.
        Internalised stigma in people living with chronic pain.
        J Pain. 2014; 15: 550.e1-550.e10
        • Williams A.C.
        Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2002; 25 (discussion 455-488): 439-455
        • Williams A.C.
        • Craig K.D.
        Updating the definition of pain.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 2420-2423
        • Williams A.C.
        • Gallagher E.
        • Fidalgo A.R.
        • Bentley P.J.
        Pain expressiveness and altruistic behavior: An exploration using agent-based modeling.
        Pain. 2016; 157: 759-768
        • Zdaniuk A.
        • Bobocel D.R.
        Vertical individualism and injustice: The self-restorative function of revenge.
        Eur J Soc Psychol. 2012; 42: 640-651