Highlights
- •There are few studies of cognitive biases and pain in children and adolescents.
- •Evidence for vigilance to pain-related stimuli in youth with chronic pain.
- •No convincing evidence of biases in other attentional processes exists.
- •Youth with chronic pain seem to interpret ambiguous stimuli as pain related.
Abstract
This review investigated whether youth exhibit attention or interpretation biases
toward pain-related information and whether such biases are more pronounced in youth
with chronic pain. Three databases were searched to identify studies that assessed
attention or interpretation biases using an accepted experimental paradigm. Ten studies
were identified, 8 examining attentional biases and 2 examining interpretation biases.
As in the adult literature, there was no evidence of attentional biases toward pain
in youth without chronic pain. Three studies investigating youth without chronic pain
found evidence for relationships between catastrophizing or anxiety and indicators
of vigilance or avoidance (in 2 cases, for youth with low self-reported attentional
control). For attentional biases, 5 studies compared youth with and without chronic
pain. Two of these studies measured cortical correlates and found evidence of neurologic
activity indicating a bias in orienting to pain-related stimuli. Three studies examined
biases toward pain-related words or pictures. Of those, 2 found evidence of biases
at subliminal presentation times, indicating vigilance (although 1 only after a stressful
task). For supraliminal presentations, 1 study found evidence of avoidance, one of
difficulty disengaging, and one of general slowing of responses. Only 1 study compared
youth with and without pain for interpretation bias in adolescents, and interpretation
biases were greater for youth with chronic pain. As with attention, no evidence for
interpretation biases were found in youth without chronic pain. Overall, there is
weak evidence to support vigilance in youth with chronic pain compared with those
without. However, whether pain affects the subsequent deployment of attention is unclear.
There is no evidence for biases toward pain in youth without chronic pain, but evidence
suggests that anxiety or catastrophizing and attentional control may moderate pain-related
attentional biases. There is also weak evidence of interpretation bias in youth with
chronic pain compared with those without.
Perspective
Children without chronic pain do not show interpretation or attention biases toward
pain-related stimuli. However, there is weak evidence for the presence of attention
biases, characterized by vigilance toward pain-related stimuli and pain-related interpretation
bias in children with chronic pain compared with those without.
Key words
To read this article in full you will need to make a payment
Purchase one-time access:
Academic & Personal: 24 hour online accessCorporate R&D Professionals: 24 hour online accessOne-time access price info
- For academic or personal research use, select 'Academic and Personal'
- For corporate R&D use, select 'Corporate R&D Professionals'
Subscribe:
Subscribe to The Journal of PainAlready a print subscriber? Claim online access
Already an online subscriber? Sign in
Register: Create an account
Institutional Access: Sign in to ScienceDirect
References
- Pediatric fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: foundation, application and future directions.Pain Res Manage. 2012; 17: 397-405
- Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study.Psychol Bull. 2007; 133: 1
- Attentional bias to pain and social threat in pediatric patients with functional abdominal pain and pain-free youth before and after performance evaluation.Pain. 2011; 152: 1061-1067
- Attentional biases to pain and social threat in children with recurrent abdominal pain.J Pediatr Psychol. 2006; 31: 209-220
- Scientific guidelines for conducting integrative research reviews.Review of Educational Research. 1982; 52: 291-302
- Practitioner review: cognitive bias modification for mental health problems in children and adolescents: a meta‐analysis.J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015; 56: 723-734
- Attentional bias to pain-related information: a meta-analysis.Pain. 2013; 154: 497-510
- Pain-related attentional biases: the importance of the personal relevance and ecological validity of stimuli.J Pain. 2011; 12: 625-632
- The psychometric properties of the dot-probe paradigm when used in pain-related attentional bias research.J Pain. 2011; 12: 1247-1254
- Attentional bias towards threatening stimuli in children with anxiety: a meta-analysis.Clin Psychol Rev. 2015; 40: 66-75
- Word completion in Chronic Pain: Evidence for Schematic representation of pain.J Abnorm Psychol. 1994; 103: 379-382
- Is there room for ‘development’ in developmental models of information processing biases to threat in children and adolescents?.Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2010; 13: 315-332
- Frontal lobe and cognitive development.J Neurocytol. 2002; 31: 373-385
- Information Processing across three chronic pain groups.in: Abstracts of 8th World Congress on Pain. Seattle, WA, International Association for the Study of Pain. 1996: 75
- Biased interpretations of ambiguous bodily threat information in adolescents with chronic pain.Pain. 2017; 158: 471-478
- Attention bias modification training for adolescents with chronic pain: a randomized placebo controlled trial.Pain. 2017; (Oct 30 [Epub ahead of print])
- Negative interpretation bias and the experience of pain in adolescents.J Pain. 2016; 17: 972-981
- Child attention to pain and pain tolerance are dependent upon anxiety and attention control: an eye-tracking study.European Journal of Pain (United Kingdom). 2017; 21: 250-263
- The relationship between adolescents' pain catastrophizing and attention bias to pain faces is moderated by attention control.Pain. 2015; 156: 1334-1341
- Sex differences in pain intensity in adolescents suffering from cancer: differences in pain memories?.J Pain. 2009; 10: 586-593
- Cortical correlates of an attentional bias to painful and innocuous somatic stimuli in children with recurrent abdominal pain.Pain. 2008; 136: 397-406
- Cognitive bias modification: A review of meta-analyses.J Affect Disord. 2017; 223: 175-183
- Interpretation biases in chronic pain patients: An incidental learning task.Eur J Pain. 2015; 19:1139-1147
- Children in pain: an investigation of selective memory for pain and psychological adjustment.Pain. 1999; 81: 237-244
- The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: current state of scientific evidence.J Behav Med. 2007; 30: 77-94
- Using eye movements to investigate selective attention in chronic daily headache.Pain. 2014; 155: 503-510
- Contribution of attentional and cognitive factors to laser evoked brain potentials.Neurophysiol Clin. 2003; 33: 293-301
- Attentional bias in emotional disorders.J Abnorm Psychol. 1986; 95: 15-20
- The effect of attentional re-training and threat expectancy in response to acute pain.Pain. 2009; 142: 101-107
- Remembering the hurt of childhood: A psychological review and call for future research.Can Psychol. 2017; 58: 58
- Remembering pain after surgery: A longitudinal examination of the role of pain catastrophizing in children's and parents' recall.Pain. 2015; 156: 800-808
- Interpretation bias in responses to ambiguous cues in pain patients.J Psychosom Res. 1994; 38: 347-353
- Pain patients' bias in the interpretation of ambiguous homophones.Br J Med Psychol. 1996; 69: 259-266
- Biased interpretation of ambiguous information in patients with chronic pain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of current studies.Health Psychol. 2016; 35: 944-956
- Attentional bias towards pain-related information in chronic pain; a meta-analysis of visual-probe investigations.Clin Psychol Rev. 2012; 32: 13-25
- Is there a potential role for attention bias modification in pain patients? Results of 2 randomised, controlled trials.Pain. 2012; 153: 722-731
- Attention bias modification and its impact on experimental pain outcomes: Comparison of training with words versus faces in pain.Eur J Pain. 2015; 19: 1248-1257
- Attention bias modification for children: What should we make of a null finding?.Pain. 2017; (Nov 13 [Epub ahead of print])
- Threat interpretation and parental influences for children with asthma and anxiety.Behav Res Ther. 2017; 89: 14-23
- The Fear Avoidance model of chronic pain: Examination for pediatric application.J Pain. 2012; 13: 827-835
- Comparison of average weekly pain using recalled paper and momentary assessment electronic diary reports in children with arthritis.Clin J Pain. 2014; 30: 1044-1050
- Towards a new model of attentional biases in the development, maintenance, and management of pain.Pain. 2015; 156: 1589-1600
- The occurrence of recall bias in pediatric headache: a comparison of questionnaire and diary data.Headache. 2001; 41: 11-20
- Attentional bias to activity of different parts of the body in children with functional abdominal pain: An experimental study.J Pediatr Psychol. 2014; 39: 438-449
- Children's selective attention to pain and avoidance behaviour: The role of child and parental catastrophizing about pain.Pain. 2013; 154: 1979-1988
- The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology.Psychol Bull. 1996; 120: 3-24
- Identifying selective visual attention biases related to fear of pain by tracking eye movements within a dot-probe paradigm.Pain. 2012; 153: 1742-1748
- Altered pain processing in children with migraine: An evoked potential study.Eur J Pain. 2008; 12: 1090-1101
Article info
Publication history
Published online: May 04, 2018
Accepted:
April 18,
2018
Received in revised form:
March 15,
2018
Received:
November 7,
2017
Footnotes
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Identification
Copyright
Crown Copyright © 2018 Published by Elsevier Inc. on behalf of the American Pain Society